FreshRSS

๐Ÿ”’
โŒ About FreshRSS
There are new available articles, click to refresh the page.
Before yesterdayYour RSS feeds

Without oxytocin receptors, voles are just as lovey-dovey

two hearts with googley eyes

The standard understanding of oxytocin, colloquially known as the โ€œlove hormone,โ€ may be all wrong.

Researchers report surprising results from a study that turns a dogma concerning the biology of friendliness, romantic attachment, and parental probity on its head.

They found that stifling the activity of oxytocin in prairie voles doesnโ€™t change these rodentsโ€™ behaviorโ€”they remain the monogamous, protective parents theyโ€™ve always been.

โ€œAs you can imagine, this is huge,โ€ says Nirao Shah, professor of psychiatry and behavioral sciences and of neurobiology at the Stanford University School of Medicine.

Long considered important in mammalian pair bonding and good parenting, oxytocin has been stewarded into numerous clinical trials on the assumption that, administered as a drug, it might encourage sociability in people with conditions such as schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorder.

The new study in Neuron seriously challenges that assumption.

Oxytocin signaling

Hormones are chemical messengers that influence the way cells act. (And multi-celled organisms such as humans are, after all, composed of cells.) Hormones circulate throughout the body doing various things from regulating sugar uptake and sodium retention to sex drive and the menstrual cycle. In every case, they work by latching on to molecular receptors.

Different hormones latch on to different receptors. Some cells have receptors for hormone A or hormone B; others donโ€™t. A cell without a matching receptor for a given hormone has no way of knowing a hormone is in the neighborhood, much less which one it is. So, it wonโ€™t respond to that hormone.

Oxytocin, too, is understood to act by binding to its own dedicated receptor, a cell-surface protein that abounds in distinct structures in the brain.

Previous studies of oxytocinโ€™s activity have largely relied on drugs that block or mimic the moleculeโ€™s binding to its receptor. But drugs can be notoriously imprecise in space and time, Shah says. They can diffuse to areas unrelated to the one under study, and they can persist in a tissue or on a cell surface for longer than scientists intend, producing a spurious result. And they can trip off, or inhibit, activities in cells that werenโ€™t meant to be part of the experiment. Theyโ€™re never 100% perfect at mimicking the action of the substance theyโ€™re supposed to mirror.

โ€œWe were curious about whether oxytocin signaling in fact mediated all the various aspects of social attachment and other behaviors attributed to it, because most of the work had been done with pharmacological agents,โ€ Shah says.

He and his colleagues turned to a more meticulous genetic technology. They used CRISPRโ€”an extremely precise molecular editing technique used to snip out stretches of DNAโ€”to delete the gene for the oxytocin receptor in prairie voles.

Prairie vole families

Not all that many animals are doting parents, mind you. (Scorpions sometimes eat their young.) And very few are monogamous. But as a species, prairie voles serve as the ultimate commercial for lifetime marriage and nuclear families. Couples, once bonded, stick together for the long run. They huddle over their pups to keep them warm, and they retrieve any of their offspring who wander too far from the nest and into harmโ€™s way. Plus, these fuzzy, nuzzling natives of the US Midwest are quite cute.

The investigatorsโ€™ experiments showed that long after mating, pairs of voles that lacked oxytocin receptors continued to hang out together. Not only that, but they displayed outright aggression toward opposite-sexed strangers of the same speciesโ€”the opposite of what would be expected of a non-monogamous animal of any species. Moms happily nursed their newborns. Dads chipped in, helping keep the pups warm, clean, and close by.

In short, prairie voles whose oxytocin receptors are non-existent still exhibit monogamous behavior and conscientious co-parenting. Apparently, oxytocin signaling through its receptor isnโ€™t required for this loving behavior, as has been assumed, Shah says.

In fact, no result from the studyโ€”which was 15 years in the makingโ€”indicated a critical role for the oxytocin receptor in pro-social behavior (at least, not in prairie voles) or, by extension, for oxytocin.

Either oxytocin just plain isnโ€™t essential to the cuddly camaraderie long attributed to it, or maybe in the total absence of this receptor, compensatory brain circuitry arose in early development to step up and take on pair-bonding and parenting responsibilities.

Or could it be that this hallowed hormone is two-timing its acknowledged receptor via covert liaisons with some mysterious, as-yet-unidentified receptor hiding in plain sight?

Itโ€™s also possible that some other, more relevant biological pathwayโ€”perhaps involving another hormone such as vasopressin, which is structurally and physiologically similar to oxytocinโ€”may be underlying all these pro-social effects attributed to oxytocin.

In any case, the studyโ€™s findings may explain why several clinical trials of oxytocin as a treatment for alleviating social symptoms of autism or schizophrenia have generated mixed or downright disappointing results in the past year or so, Shah suggests.

โ€œIt looks as though people may have been barking up the wrong tree,โ€ he says.

Source: Stanford University

The post Without oxytocin receptors, voles are just as lovey-dovey appeared first on Futurity.

โŒ