FreshRSS

๐Ÿ”’
โŒ About FreshRSS
There are new available articles, click to refresh the page.
Before yesterdayYour RSS feeds

Little kids can tell good excuses from bad ones

A little boy looks up at the camera with a skeptical expression.

When it comes to broken promises, children as young as 3 to 5 recognize that some excuses are better than others.

โ€œAt 3 to 5 years old, kids are on to you. They know when youโ€™re giving a bad excuse,โ€ says first author Leon Li, who did the research with developmental psychologist Michael Tomasello as part of his PhD in psychology and neuroscience at Duke University.

For the study in the journal Cognitive Development, 64 3- and 5-year-olds were shown a series of videos in which puppets promised to show them a cool toy, left the scene to go get it, but then came back empty-handed.

Afterwards, the puppets either gave a good excuse for going back on their word (โ€œI had to help my friend with his homeworkโ€), a bad excuse (โ€œI wanted to watch TVโ€), or no explanation at all. The children were then asked whether they thought the puppetsโ€™ actions were wrong or not, and why.

No matter what the excuse (or lack thereof), the children agreed that it was generally wrong to break a promise. But they were more understanding when the puppets offered a good excuse (i.e., they had to help someone), versus a bad one (i.e., they just wanted to do something fun instead).

In other words, children this age grasp that obligations to help others take priority over selfish desires, Li says.

The childrenโ€™s responses also revealed that a poor excuse was just as bad as none at all.

โ€œPrevious research has suggested that in some cases, young kids will just take any reason to be better than no reason at all,โ€ Li says. โ€œBut here we showed that kids do pay attention to the actual content.โ€

When asked to explain their answers, the childrenโ€™s justifications changed with age. Compared with 3-year-olds, 5-year-olds were better at articulating their thinking in terms of what the puppets โ€œshouldโ€ do, or are โ€œsupposedโ€ to do, suggesting that kidsโ€™ understanding of obligations to others is more fully developed by this age.

Surprisingly, lousy excuses didnโ€™t make the children less inclined to say they โ€œlikedโ€ some puppets, or would invite those puppets to a play date.

โ€œUsually if someone breaks a promise and gives you a [bad] reason, it implies theyโ€™re not really a good friend,โ€ Li says. โ€œChildren this age donโ€™t make that connection. Theyโ€™re just not there yet.โ€

This study is part of a larger field of research on how children come to appreciate and act on cultural and moral norms for how we behave and treat each other.

โ€œMorality is a type of common ground that we have with others, with mutual expectations about how we should behave and what counts as good grounds for justification,โ€ Li says.

โ€œWeโ€™re showing that young children become attuned to this common ground at an early age.โ€

Li says the findings are also relevant to any adult who has uttered the classic fallback phrase, โ€œBecause I said so.โ€

โ€œKids are paying attention and can tell that is a [bad] reason,โ€ Li says.

Support for the research came the Duke psychology and neuroscience department.

Source: Duke University

The post Little kids can tell good excuses from bad ones appeared first on Futurity.

โŒ